Fishes feigning death
StŽphan G. Reebs
UniversitŽ de Moncton,
Canada
2007
Possums are not the only animals that Òplay possumÓ. Acting as though
dead has been observed in insects, frogs, reptiles (especially snakes), birds
(a lot of studies have been done on chickens), and mammals (squirrels, foxes,
and, of course, possums). The behaviour is usually expressed when the animal is
caught by a predator, or firmly grabbed by a person. The animal goes limp and
apparently lifeless. It can be prodded without eliciting any reaction. Yet the
animal has not fainted, because neurobiological recordings show that the brain
of an animal feigning death is just as active as when fully conscious. And
indeed, if the predator or person moves away, the animal ÒmiraculouslyÓ revives
and escapes.
To explain why this behaviour has evolved, it has often been pointed out
that many predators stop mauling their prey when they can plainly see that it
is dead, and in the ensuing moments of inattention, or if the greedy predator temporarily
moves away to catch other victims, the faker might make good its escape. It is
a reasonable explanation, but we still donÕt know if it is true. Predatory acts
are hard to witness in nature, and those that involve death-feigning prey are
harder still. Many researchers abstain from using the expression
Òdeath-feigningÓ – they say it implies a function that has not been
proven. Instead they resort to the more clinical terms Òtonic immobilityÓ,
ÒthanatosisÓ, or Òanimal hypnosisÓ.
Death feigning is not unknown in the fish world. Hereafter are a few
examples.
Tonic immobility in sharks and other elasmobranches
Next time you get a chance, grab a shark, turn it onto its back, and
restrain it by hand. Chances are that after a few seconds (usually less than a
minute) the shark will go limp and start breathing smoothly and deeply. It will
remain in this cataleptic state for several minutes. If left alone, the fish will
eventually right itself and swim away. Such a response has been observed in
many species of cartilaginous fish, such as the tiger shark, lemon shark,
smooth dogfish, spiny dogfish, leopard shark, whitetip reef shark, blacktip
reef shark, Caribbean reef shark, swellshark, sandbar shark, California round
ray, shovelnose guitarfish, clearnose skate, cownose ray, and southern stingray.[1]
Tonic immobility in sharks is such a reliable behaviour that it can be
used as a form of anaesthesia, either in the field[2]
or in aquaria,[3] prior to
minor surgery or gross examination. However, caution must be used in the case
of some species that experience adverse effects when the tonic immobility is maintained
for too long.[4]
Why sharks should go limp when they are inverted is unclear. The
classical explanation of playing possum in front of a predator has been put forward,
as has the notion that immobility during copulation (often a violent affair in
sharks) might expedite things.
Tonic immobility in bony fishes
Many bony fishes also go limp when they are restrained on their backs:
goldfish,[5]
trout, rudd, tench, brown bullhead, medaka, paradise fish, and topminnow, to
name a few.[6]
Some anglers use this response to good effect: when removing a hook from a fish
that is to be released, they invert the fish and tuck it under one arm, thereby
quieting it. A lip grip can also immobilize some fishes.
Oscars (Astronotus ocellatus) are big predatory fish, but sometimes they
appear downright wimpy. When stressed (when you clean their aquarium, for
example), they seem to go into shock. They lie on their side, stop moving their
fins, start to breathe more slowly and deeply, and lose colour. Maybe they
simply become inactive to elude the attention of a perceived predator, but the
fact that they remain motionless even when nudged by hand suggests tonic
immobility.[7]
Convict tangs, Acanthurus triostegus, show a similar behaviour in the field.[8]
A new way to induce tonic immobility
In 2005, researchers Rufus Wells, Heather McNeil and John MacDonald,
from the University of Auckland in New Zealand, reported a new method for
inducing tonic immobility in fish. When they inserted a tube into the buccal
cavity and forced a vigorous flow of water through the gills, they observed
that all fish from all of the 22 species they tested went limp and remained
that way for as long as the flow of water was kept up (up to 32 h in one case).
Revival was instantaneous once the flow was stopped, and the fish seemed none
the worse for wear. The species included short-finned eels, kahawai, leather
jackets, lowly trevally, blue maomao, epaulette shark, blue cod, sand flounder,
rainbow trout, and slimy mackerel. No particular advantage for the fish comes
to mind, but perhaps the technique could be used to calm fish down during live
transport.[9]
Death-feigning as a hunting strategy
Some bestiaries from medieval times depict a fox lying on its side, its
eyes closed, its lower jaw hanging open, looking dead. It is surrounded by
scavenging birds. The next illustration shows the fox leaping up and catching
one of the birds. The possibility that foxes would be cunning enough to sham
death in order to lure birds within reach was originally dismissed as a folk
tale, but in 1961 a Russian film-maker managed to record a fox in the act. A
crow was the hapless victim.[10]
For a while this remained the only example of death-feigning as a confirmed
hunting strategy in the animal world. Then in 1981 Ken McKaye published a paper
on the predatory cichlid Nimbochromis (= Haplochromis) livingstonii. Diving in Lake Malawi, McKaye
observed some N. livingstonii that were stationary with their abdomen on or near sand, and that then
dropped onto their sides. In a variant behaviour, some N. livingstonii fell through the water column and
landed onto their side. The fish then remained immobile for several minutes.
Their colour pattern was blotchy and suggested a rotting carcass. Small
inquisitive cichlids of other species often came near (a few going so far as to
nip the ÒcorpseÓ) and they were suddenly attacked by the deceitful predator.
About a third of the death-feigning performances led to an attack, and about
one-sixth of the attacks were successful. After an attack the predators moved
on to other fish aggregations nearby and played dead again. On average,
individual N. livingstonii feigned death 7 times per 30-minute watch.[11]
In 2005, death feigning as a hunting strategy was reported in another
cichlid, the yellow jacket Parachromis friedrichsthalii. Working with a population that
lived in a cenote (a water-filled sinkhole) on the Yucatan Peninsula, Michael
Tobler observed many individual P. friedrichsthalii that repeatedly turned over onto
their sides at the bottom of the sinkhole and remained immobile for as long as
15 minutes. Twice, Tobler witnessed an attack on small mollies that came too
close to the head of the pseudo cadavers.[12]
The comb grouper Mycteroperca acutirostris may also be an actor, though in
this case the behaviour should be called dying or illness feigning, rather than
death feigning, because while lying on its side the fish occasionally undulates
its body. In 1999, off the coast of southeastern Brazil, Fernando Gibran saw
one juvenile comb grouper use this tactic to catch 5 small prey in 15 minutes.
The behaviour must be uncommon, however, as only one juvenile out of 522
observed over 60 h of diving ever performed it. Perhaps the fish was truly sick, but not sick enough to refuse
a passing snack.[13]
[1] Whitman, P.A., Marshall, J.A., and
Keller, E.C.Jr., 1986, Tonic immobility in the smooth dogfish shark, Mustelus
canis (Pisces,
Carcharhinidae), Copeia 1986, 829-832; Watsky, M.A., and Gruber, S.H., 1990,
Induction and duration of tonic immobility in the lemon shark, Negaprion
brevirostris, Fish
Physiology and Biochemistry 8, 207-210; Davie, P.S., Franklin, C.E., and Grigg,
G.C., 1993, Blood pressure and heart rate during tonic immobility in the black
tipped reef shark, Carcharhinus melanoptera, Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 12, 95-100;
Henningsen, A.D., 1994, Tonic immobility in 12 elasmobranchs : Use as an
aid in captive husbandry, Zoo Biology 13, 325-332.
[2] Heithaus, M.R., Dill, L.M.,
Marshall, G.J., and Buhleier, B., 2002, Habitat use and foraging behavior of
tiger sharks (Galeocerdo cuvier) in a seagrass ecosystem, Marine Biology 140, 237-248; Holland, K.N.,
Wetherbee, B.M., owe, C.G., and Meyer, C.G., 1999, Movements of tiger sharks (Galeocerdo
cuvier) in coastal
Hawaiian waters, Marine Biology 134, 665-673; Watsky, M.A., and Gruber, S.H.,
1990, Induction and duration of tonic immobility in the lemon shark, Negaprion
brevirostris, Fish
Physiology and Biochemistry 8, 207-210.
[3] Henningsen, A.D., 1994, Tonic
immobility in 12 elasmobranchs : Use as an aid in captive husbandry, Zoo
Biology 13, 325-332.
[4] Murray, M.J., 2002, Fish surgery,
Seminars in avian and exotic pet medicine 11, 246-257.
[5] Richardson, E.J., Shumaker, M.J.,
and Harvey, E.R., 1977, The effects of stimulus presentation during cataleptic,
restrained, and free swimming states on avoidance conditioning of goldfish (Carassius
auratus), The
Psychological Record 27, 63-75; Lefebvre, L., and Sabourin, M., 1977, Effects
of spaced and massed repeated elicitation on tonic immobility in the goldfish (Carassius
auratus),
Behavioral Biology 21, 300-305.
[6] See Table 2 in: Whitman, P.A.,
Marshall, J.A., and Keller, E.C.Jr., 1986, Tonic immobility in the smooth
dogfish shark, Mustelus canis (Pisces, Carcharhinidae), Copeia 1986, 829-832.
[7] IÕve seen my own oscars do this.
For a more formal study, see: Crawford, F.T., 1977, Induction and duration of
tonic immobility, The Psychological Record 27, 89-107.
[8] Howe, J.C., 1991, Field observations of death
feigning in the convict tang, Acanthurus triostegus (Linnaeus), with comments on the
nocturnal color pattern in juvenile specimens, Journal of Aquariculture and
Aquatic Sciences 6: 13-15.
[9] Wells, R.M.G., McNeil, H., and
MacDonald, J.A., 2005, Fish hypnosis: induction of an atonic immobility reflex,
Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 38, 71-78.
[10] Page 82 in: Morris, D., 1990,
Animalwatching: A field guide to animal behaviour, Jonathan Cape, London.
[11] McKaye, K.R., 1981, Field
observation on death feigning: a unique hunting behavior by the predatory
cichlid, Haplochromis livingstonii, of Lake Malawi, Environmental Biology of
Fishes 6, 361-365. Another African cichlid, Lamprologus lemairii, from Lake Tanganyika, has been
reported to do the same thing: Lucanus, O., 1998, DarwinÕs pond: Malawi and
Tanganyika, Tropical Fish Hobbyist 47, 150-154.
[12] Tobler, M., 2005, Feigning death in
the Central American cichlid Parachromis friedrichsthalii, Journal of Fish Biology 66,
877-881.
[13] Gibran, F.Z., 2004, Dying or
illness feigning : An unreported feeding tactic of the comb grouper Mycteroperca
acutirostris
(Serranidae) from the Southwest Atlantic, Copeia 2004, 403-405.